Warning: file_put_contents(/opt/frankenphp/design.onmedianet.com/storage/proxy/cache/725144230dad54b9b37e9fb55d2e9cbb.html): Failed to open stream: No space left on device in /opt/frankenphp/design.onmedianet.com/app/src/Arsae/CacheManager.php on line 36

Warning: http_response_code(): Cannot set response code - headers already sent (output started at /opt/frankenphp/design.onmedianet.com/app/src/Arsae/CacheManager.php:36) in /opt/frankenphp/design.onmedianet.com/app/src/Models/Response.php on line 17

Warning: Cannot modify header information - headers already sent by (output started at /opt/frankenphp/design.onmedianet.com/app/src/Arsae/CacheManager.php:36) in /opt/frankenphp/design.onmedianet.com/app/src/Models/Response.php on line 20
Deficits in recognizing female facial expressions related to social network in cocaine-addicted men - PMC Skip to main content
UKPMC Funders Author Manuscripts logoLink to UKPMC Funders Author Manuscripts
. Author manuscript; available in PMC: 2024 Dec 8.
Published in final edited form as: Drug Alcohol Depend. 2020 Aug 27;216:108247. doi: 10.1016/j.drugalcdep.2020.108247

Deficits in recognizing female facial expressions related to social network in cocaine-addicted men

Amy R Bland a,b, Karen D Ersche a,*
PMCID: PMC7616971  EMSID: EMS118372  PMID: 32896724

Abstract

Background

The ability to accurately recognise facial expressions of emotion is crucial for social functioning and maintaining healthy relationships. Recognising the emotional state of others allows us to respond to their needs and adjust our behaviour appropriately. Impairments in facial affect recognition have been reported in chronic cocaine users but little is known whether these contribute to their difficulties in social situations.

Methods

We assessed facial emotional expression recognition in forty-five men with cocaine use disorder (CUD) and forty-four healthy control participants. Using standardised questionnaires, we also collected information on perceived social support, social provision and community integration.

Results

Our results found that male cocaine users had greater difficulty in recognising female emotional facial expressions than male controls. This effect was not explained by demographic variables but it was associated with their social network; including social support, social provisions and community integration.

Conclusion

Our findings suggest that men with CUD have greater difficulty in identifying emotional expression in female faces, which is linked with their social support networks. This may play an important role in misunderstanding non-verbal communications that contribute to destabilising friendship and family ties typically seen in drug addiction. Addressing deficits in recognising female emotional expressions may be an important piece of information for counselling and other interventions.

Keywords: Fear, Disgust, Cocaine Use Disorder, Addiction, Emotion, Social cognition

1. Introduction

The emotional toll of drug addiction, which includes relationship break-ups and breaks with family ties that even lead to social exclusion, remains immeasurable. Whilst much research has been focussed on understanding the effects of drugs to curtail further use, relatively less is known how chronic drug use affect social functioning and how this knowledge could be used improve psychosocial interventions. For example, accumulating evidence indicates impairments in recognising emotional facial expressions (Ersche et al., 2015; Kuypers et al., 2015; Morgan and Marshall, 2013; Verdejo-García and Bechara, 2009; Kemmis et al., 2007) and deficits in social cognition, including perspective-taking and social decision-making (Quednow, 2017; Romero-Martínez and Moya-Albiol, 2015; Preller et al., 2014). As facial expressions reflect the emotional state of others, enabling us to adapt our behaviour appropriately to other people’s need, failure to recognise facial affect has been shown to be predictive of a pattern of social cognitive deficits (Trubanova et al., 2016; Corden et al., 2006). Specifically, failure to recognise negative emotions, such as fear, have been linked with an inability to inhibit socially inappropriate behaviours (Blair, 1995) and may create difficulties maintaining interpersonal relationships. It is thus conceivable that such deficits contribute to the difficulties frequently reported by chronic cocaine users such as maintaining healthy and rewarding relationships (Pachado et al., 2018).

Recognition of emotional expressions is largely affected by social influences such as biases towards one’s own gender (Wright and Sladden, 2003), ethnicity and culture (Lee et al., 2008; Rehnman and Herlitz, 2006). Gender-specific effects in emotion recognition have to the best of our knowledge not been investigated in cocaine addiction, although it may have far reaching consequences on drug users’ social lives. We therefore sought to first replicate previous findings showing facial affect recognition deficits in cocaine-addicted men and subsequently investigate the influence of facial gender. We hypothesised that men with chronic cocaine use show deficits in facial affect recognition, specifically in female faces. We further hypothesised that participants’ ability in recognising emotional expressions of others is associated with social aspects of their lives.

2. Methods

A total of eighty-nine male volunteers, with a mean age of 40.3 years (s.d. ± 10.7), were recruited via adverts in the local region and by word-of-mouth. Forty-five males were not seeking treatment for their cocaine use and have been using cocaine for an average of 13.2 years. Participants were assessed using the SCID for the DSM-IV-TR (Spitzer et al., 2002) and diagnoses were then followed up with DSM-5 (American Psychiatric Association, 2013) criteria for cocaine use disorder, subsequently referred to as CUD. The remaining forty-four men were healthy and without a personal and family history of drug abuse. All CUD men were active users of cocaine, as verified by a cocaine-positive urine sample prior to testing which has a detection window for cocaine of 2–3 days; urine samples provided by control participants were negative. Prior to testing, the absence of withdrawal and acute intoxication were checked. All participants were also breathalysed prior to testing to ensure no alcohol intoxication and completed the National Adult Reading Test (NART; Nelson and Willison, 1991) to estimate verbal intelligence and the Depression, Anxiety and Stress Scale (DASS-21; Lovibond and Lovibond, 1995) to assess affective states. The Multidimensional Scale of Perceived Social Support (MSPSS; Zimet et al., 1988), the Community Integration Questionnaire (CIQ-II; Sander et al., 1999) and the Social Provisions Scale (SPS; Cutrona and Russell, 1987) were used to evaluate the quality of participants’ social networks. Ninety-one percent of both groups reported being heterosexual; subsequent exclusion of non-heterosexual participants did not change the results. Whist the majority of CUD participants reported being single (82 %) and 18 % reported being divorced or separated, only 43 % of control participants reported being single, 9 % divorced or separated; the rest of the control group was married (47 %). Further details can be found in the supplemental material. The protocol was approved by the Psychological Research Ethics Committee.

We first verified participants’ ability to perceptually discriminate faces using the Benton Facial Recognition Test (Benton et al., 1983) and identified three CUD participants with severe difficulties, who were subsequently excluded from further analysis.

2.1. Emotional intensity morphing task

The Emotional Intensity Morphing Task (EIMT) of the EMOTICOM neuropsychological test battery (Bland et al., 2016), assesses the point of emotional intensity at which participants can recognize an emotional facial expression. Participants view faces that either increase or decrease in emotional intensity and are instructed to respond when they either (a) detect the presence of emotion or (b) no longer detect the presence of emotion. The task includes five different emotions: happiness, sadness, anger, fear and disgust. The point of detection was calculated by taking the level of intensity in the facial expression needed in order to detect (increasing) or no longer detect (decreasing) each emotion (see supplemental material).

2.2. Statistical analysis

Data were analysed using the Statistical Package for the Social Sciences (SPSS, V25; IBM). Group differences regarding demographics and face recognition performance were analysed using independent samples t-tests. Performance on the EIMT was analysed using repeated-measures and multivariate ANOVA models. Pearson’s correlation coefficients were used to establish relationships between participants’ task performance and social network indicators. All statistical tests were two-tailed with significance levels set at 0.05.

3. Results

As shown in Table 1, the two groups were well-matched for age but differed significantly with regard to verbal IQ, current emotional state, social support, social provisions and community integration. As differences in IQ were not correlated with task performance in CUD participants, they were not included in the analysis.

Table 1. Demographics and clinical variables for male volunteers with and without CUD.

Measure Control Group CUD Group Group Comparison
Mean SD Mean SD t P
Age (years) 40.19 12.46 40.33 8.82 -0.58 0.950
Verbal IQ (NART score) 115.49 6.14 102.53 7.29 8.45 * < 0.001
Affective state (DASS-21) 10.33 8.20 45.53 22.58 -9.61 * < 0.001
Perceived social support (MSPSS) 72.05 12.24 51.14 17.88 6.32 * < 0.001
Social Provisions (SPS) 83.00 9.49 66.05 7.92 8.93 * < 0.001
Community integration (CIQ-II) 17.60 3.16 13.83 3.96 4.86 * < 0.001

Results, depicted in Fig. 1, showed that CUD participants required significantly greater intensities to recognise facial emotion expressions [F(1,84) = 9.095,p = 0.003,ηp2 = 0.10)]. Moreover, the group-by-gender interaction [F(1,84) = 5.76,p = 0.019,ηp2 = 0.06)] revealed that CUD participants required significantly greater intensities to recognise emotional female faces [F(1,84) = 13.42,p = 0.001,ηp2 = 0.14)] but did not differ from controls in their ability to recognise emotional male faces [F(1,84) = 1.61,p = 0.208,ηp2 = 0.02)].

Fig. 1.

Fig. 1

Bar chart depicting recognition performance of male and female facial affect across five emotions. Men with CUD require greater intensity of affect in recognising happiness, fearfulness and disgust in female faces both compared with male faces and compared with intensities required by their non-drug using counterparts.

Multivariate analyses further revealed that CUD participants exhibited specific impairments in recognising happiness [F(1,84) = 7.90,p = 0.018,ηp2 = 0.09)], fearfulness [F(1,84) = 6.15,p = 0.015,ηp2 = 0.07)] and disgust in female faces [F(1,84) = 9.13,p = 0.003,ηp2 = 0.10)], whereas sadness (p = 0.477) and anger (p = 0.590) were recognised similarly to control participants. However, recognition of fearful female faces was inversely correlated with level of community integration (CIQ-II-total: r=-0.24,p = 0.026), perceived social support (MSPSS-total: r=-0.20,p = 0.007) and social provisions (SPS-total: r=-0.24,p = 0.022). When further co-varying for DASS-21 scores, recognition of fearful female faces remained correlated with level of community integration (CIQ-II-total: r=-0.21,p = 0.05) and perceived social support (MSPSS-total: r=-0.25,p = 0.022) with a trend towards social provisions (SPS-total: r=-0.20, p = 0.077). However, we found that task performance was not associated with duration of cocaine use (r=-0.09,p = 0.53), age of onset of cocaine use (r=-0.06,p = 0.69), or the compulsive pattern of cocaine use, as reflected by the obsessive-compulsive drug use (OCDUS) score (r = 0.22,p = 0.31).

4. Discussion

Here we provide novel insights into facial affect recognition impairments in cocaine-addicted men by examining the effects of gender and their association with social aspects of participants’ lives. In line with previous studies (Ersche et al., 2015; Kuypers et al., 2015; Morgan and Marshall, 2013; Verdejo-García and Bechara, 2009; Kemmis et al., 2007), we showed that CUD participants exhibit deficits in facial affect recognition. In fact, these impairments were specific to female facial expressions, which were in turn, associated with participants’ level of social integration – an observation, which to best of our knowledge has not yet been reported.

Although CUD participants were significantly impaired in recognising the facial affect of others, this effect was, however, dependent upon the gender of the emotional face. Importantly, for the recognition of male facial affect, CUD participants were not measurably different from control participants across all five emotions tested, but they showed robust deficits in recognising female facial emotions. In fact, CUD participants required significantly greater intensity to detect emotions such as happy, fear and disgust in female faces compared with their non-drug using peers. Several accounts for this effect are conceivable. First, gender differences in expression of emotion is widely recognised, possibly due to an increased proneness of women to communicate nonverbally (Eagly, 1997; Eagly and Steffen, 1984). The cognitive deficits associated with CUD may present a particular disadvantage for CUD men, as they may not benefit as much from experiences with non-verbal strategies as much as women would (Thompson and Voyer, 2014). Consequently, they require greater intensity of emotional expressions to recognise emotion in female faces. Second, although both male and female faces were presented to participants, we only recruited men into the study, which means they could use their own-gender bias for 50 % of the trials. Own-gender bias describes the advantage of recognising facial expressions in faces of one’s own gender (Herlitz and Lovén, 2013). Consequently, CUD men may have benefitted more from this bias than their non-drug using peers. Future studies should use female CUD participants to further examine this possibility. Third, facial emotion recognition has been associated with the ability to empathise with others (Besel and Yuille, 2010) – an ability that has been suggested to be impaired in CUD (Preller et al., 2014). Possibly, CUD participants, who are unsure how to interpret a female facial emotional expression might not be able to compensate using empathetic intuition, and as a result, they are more likely to make wrong inferences about other people’s emotional states. It is unclear why CUD participants did not show deficits in recognising sadness or anger in female faces.

Whilst previous research has speculated about the potential neuro-biological nature of facial affect recognition impairment in CUD, the selective deficits for female facial expressions runs counter to such views and point more strongly towards social influences. Our results support the notion that the degree to which men recognise negative facial affect in women is associated with their social support, community integration, and social provisions such as guidance, attachment and perceived social support from friends and family. Social ties are strongly related to cocaine use trajectories (Hamil-Luker et al., 2004) and social support has been shown to be protective factors against current drug use (Williams and Latkin, 2007). Therefore suggesting an important role for social networks in emotion recognition ability in CUD.

Although drug addiction is known to destabilise friendships and family ties, such deficits in non-verbal communication are likely to further exacerbate existing tensions. This is in line with previous studies suggesting that problems with negative information processing have repercussion on social functioning in cocaine addiction and can predict treatment success (Forster et al., 2017; Turner et al., 2009). Indeed, drug-induced alterations in social perception may influence social behaviour (Wardle et al., 2012). Therefore, this impairment profile is more likely to reflect an interaction between vulnerability and the effect of the drug, which may also explain why we did not find any associations between cocaine-related variables such as the duration or compulsive pattern of use and task performance. The extent to which the impairment profile observed in the present study might be caused by cocaine specifically, should be tested by pharmacological studies using cocaine or methylphenidate (which is pharmacologically very similar to cocaine; Volkow et al., 1995) in healthy volunteers. Clearly, our work requires replication and research is warranted into whether specific training may help alleviate or improve social functioning in CUD patients.

The results of the present study are also consistent with a growing body of literature suggesting that negative emotions such as fear and disgust recognition are a particularly relevant for cocaine addiction (Albein-Urios et al., 2019; Ersche et al., 2015; Morgan and Marshall, 2013; Fernández-Serrano et al., 2010; Verdejo-García and Bechara, 2009; Kemmis et al., 2007). Facial expressions of these two emotions have been suggested to convey social signs of inhibition and avoidance – functions that are known to be impaired in CUD individuals (Ersche et al., 2016; Morein-Zamir and Robbins, 2015). Recognition of both fear and disgust is an adaptive function involving the ability to anticipate dangers, signal aversion and consequently trigger protective behaviours. CUD men did not exhibit complete impairment of these emotions with impairments only evident in recognising female faces. Specific deficits in fear and disgust recognition in female faces may therefore interfere with social functioning and lead to difficulties with relationships. Indeed, this was also reflected by fewer CUD participants being in a close relationship compared to controls.

We acknowledge a number of limitations of the present study such as the use of only male participants. It still remains unclear whether women with CUD would exhibit any gender biases in facial emotion recognition. Second, CUD and control participants significantly differed in IQ and emotional states, which is not unusual for this group (Buckley et al., 2001; Crum et al., 1993). Third, complementary measures such as theory of mind and empathy measures were not obtained and would be useful in elucidating the underlying processes in this observed gender bias as would neuroimaging data. Importantly, whilst facial emotion recognition has a neurological basis (Sprengelmeyer et al., 1998), neurological change is unlikely to explain a gender bias. Nor can it be explained by culture or age biases as there were no observed cultural differences between CUD and control participants, who came from the same local community and age was not significantly correlated with the outcome variables. Finally, given that our CUD sample were either single or separated, future research is needed to explore the relationship between gender biases in emotion recognition and links specifically with interpersonal relationships. This is particularly important considering that relationships are known to be stabilising factors during recovery and potential treatments should address these links.

In conclusion, the present study provides novel evidence of gender specific deficits in facial emotion recognition in CUD men. Specific impairments in recognising fear in female faces may provide a potential marker of social integration and functioning, which would be important for informing recovery interventions.

Supplementary Material

Supplementary Materials

Acknowledgements

We would like to thank all our volunteers, without whom this study would not have been possible. We are particularly grateful to the invaluable help of Tsen Vei Lim, Eva Groot, Ibtisam Abdi and Roderick Lumsden for data collection and data extraction respectively.

Funding support

This study received financial support from the Wellcome Trust (105602/Z/14/Z) and the NIHR Cambridge Biomedical Research Centre Cambridge.

Role of the funder/sponsor

The funding body and the sponsor (University of Cambridge) had no role in study design; in the collection, analysis and interpretation of data; in the writing of the report; and in the decision to submit the article for publication.

Footnotes

Author contribution

Obtained funding, concept and design: KDE.

Data analysis and interpretation: ARB, KDE.

Writing of the manuscript: ARB, KDE.

Critical revision of the manuscript for important intellectual content: ARB, KDE.

Approved the final article: ARB, KDE.

Declaration of Competing Interest

The authors report no declarations of interest.

References

  1. Albein-Urios N, Martinez-Gonzalez JM, Lozano-Rojas O, Verdejo-Garcia A. Dysfunctional personality beliefs linked to emotion recognition deficits in individuals with cocaine addiction and personality disorders. Front Psychiatry. 2019;10:431. doi: 10.3389/fpsyt.2019.00431. [DOI] [PMC free article] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  2. American Psychiatric Association. Diagnostic and Statistical Manual of Mental Disorders. 5th. Arlington, VA: 2013. [Google Scholar]
  3. Benton AL, Sivan PA, de S, Hamsher K, Varney D, Spreen N, Sivan AB, Vareny NR, Spreen O, Varney S. Facial Recognition: Stimulus and Multiple Choice Pictures. 1983 [Google Scholar]
  4. Besel LDS, Yuille JC. Individual differences in empathy: the role of facial expression recognition. Pers Individ Dif. 2010;49:107–112. [Google Scholar]
  5. Blair RJ. A cognitive developmental approach to mortality: investigating the psychopath. Cognition. 1995;57:1–29. doi: 10.1016/0010-0277(95)00676-p. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  6. Bland AR, Roiser JP, Mehta MA, Schei T, Boland H, Campbell-Meiklejohn DK, Emsley RA, Munafo MR, Penton-Voak IS, Seara-Cardoso A, Viding E, et al. EMOTICOM: A Neuropsychological Test Battery to Evaluate Emotion, Motivation, Impulsivity, and Social Cognition. Front Behav Neurosci. 2016 doi: 10.3389/fnbeh.2016.00025. [DOI] [PMC free article] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  7. Buckley TC, Parker JD, Heggie J. A psychometric evaluation of the BDI-II in treatment-seeking substance abusers. J Subst Abuse Treat. 2001;20:197–204. doi: 10.1016/s0740-5472(00)00169-0. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  8. Corden B, Critchley HD, Skuse D, Dolan RJ. Fear recognition ability predicts differences in social cognitive and neural functioning in men. J Cogn Neurosci. 2006;18:889–897. doi: 10.1162/jocn.2006.18.6.889. [DOI] [PMC free article] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  9. Crum RM, Helzer JE, Anthony JC. Level of education and alcohol abuse and dependence in adulthood: a further inquiry. Am J Public Health. 1993;83:830–837. doi: 10.2105/ajph.83.6.830. [DOI] [PMC free article] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  10. Cutrona CE, Russell DW. The provisions of social relationships and adaptation to stress. Adv Pers Relationships. 1987;1:37–67. [Google Scholar]
  11. Eagly AH. Sex differences in social behavior: Comparing social role theory and evolutionary psychology. American Psychologist. 1997;52(12):1380–1383. doi: 10.1037/0003-066x.52.12.1380.b. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  12. Eagly AH, Steffen VJ. Gender stereotypes stem from the distribution of women and men into social roles. J Pers Soc Psychol. 1984;46:735. [Google Scholar]
  13. Ersche KD, Hagan CC, Smith DG, Jones PS, Calder AJ, Williams GB. In the face of threat: neural and endocrine correlates of impaired facial emotion recognition in cocaine dependence. Transl Psychiatry. 2015;5:e570–e570. doi: 10.1038/tp.2015.58. [DOI] [PMC free article] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  14. Ersche KD, Gillan CM, Jones PS, Williams GB, Ward LHE, Luijten M, de Wit S, Sahakian BJ, Bullmore ET, Robbins TW. Carrots and sticks fail to change behavior in cocaine addiction. Science (80) 2016;352:1468–1471. doi: 10.1126/science.aaf3700. [DOI] [PMC free article] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  15. Fernández-Serrano MJ, Lozano Ó, Pérez-García M, Verdejo-García A. Impact of severity of drug use on discrete emotions recognition in polysubstance abusers. Drug Alcohol Depend. 2010;109:57–64. doi: 10.1016/j.drugalcdep.2009.12.007. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  16. Forster SE, Finn PR, Brown JW. Neural responses to negative outcomes predict success in community-based substance use treatment. Addiction. 2017;112:884–896. doi: 10.1111/add.13734. [DOI] [PMC free article] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  17. Hamil-Luker J, Land KC, Blau J. Diverse trajectories of cocaine use through early adulthood among rebellious and socially conforming youth. Soc Sci Res. 2004;33:300–321. doi: 10.1016/s0049-089x(03)00060-7. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  18. Herlitz A, Lovén J. Sex differences and the own-gender bias in face recognition: a meta-analytic review. Vis Cogn. 2013;21:1306–1336. [Google Scholar]
  19. Kemmis L, Hall JK, Kingston R, Morgan MJ. Impaired fear recognition in regular recreational cocaine users. Psychopharmacology (Berl) 2007;194:151–159. doi: 10.1007/s00213-007-0829-5. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  20. Kuypers KPC, Steenbergen L, Theunissen EL, Toennes SW, Ramaekers JG. Emotion recognition during cocaine intoxication. Eur Neuropsychopharmacol. 2015;25:1914–1921. doi: 10.1016/j.euroneuro.2015.08.012. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  21. Lee K-U, Khang HS, Kim K-T, Kim Y-J, Kweon Y-S, Shin Y-W, Kwon JS, Ho S-H, Garfinkel SN, Chae J-H, Liberzon I. Distinct processing of facial emotion of own-race versus other-race. Neuroreport. 2008;19 doi: 10.1097/WNR.0b013e3283052df2. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  22. Lovibond PF, Lovibond SH. The structure of negative emotional states: Comparison of the Depression Anxiety Stress Scales (DASS) with the Beck Depression and Anxiety Inventories. Behav Res Ther. 1995;33:335–343. doi: 10.1016/0005-7967(94)00075-u. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  23. Morein-Zamir S, Robbins TW. Fronto-striatal circuits in response-inhibition: relevance to addiction. Brain Res. 2015;1628:117–129. doi: 10.1016/j.brainres.2014.09.012. [DOI] [PMC free article] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  24. Morgan MJ, Marshall JP. Deficient fear recognition in regular cocaine users is not attributable to elevated impulsivity or conduct disorder prior to cocaine use. J Psychopharmacol (Oxford) 2013;27:526–532. doi: 10.1177/0269881113477708. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  25. Nelson HE, Willison J. National Adult Reading Test (NART) Nfer-Nelson Windsor; 1991. [Google Scholar]
  26. Pachado MP, Scherer JN, Guimarães LSP, von Diemen L, Pechansky F, Kessler FHP, de Almeida RMM. Markers for severity of problems in interpersonal relationships of crack cocaine users from a Brazilian multicenter study. Psychiatr Q. 2018;89:923–936. doi: 10.1007/s11126-018-9590-7. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  27. Preller KH, Hulka LM, Vonmoos M, Jenni D, Baumgartner MR, Seifritz E, Dziobek I, Quednow BB. Impaired emotional empathy and related social network deficits in cocaine users. Addict Biol. 2014;19:452–466. doi: 10.1111/adb.12070. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  28. Quednow BB. Social cognition and interaction in stimulant use disorders. Curr Opin Behav Sci. 2017;13:55–62. [Google Scholar]
  29. Rehnman J, Herlitz A. Higher face recognition ability in girls: magnified by ownsex and own-ethnicity bias. Memory. 2006;14:289–296. doi: 10.1080/09658210500233581. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  30. Romero-Martínez A, Moya-Albiol L. Neuropsychological impairments associated with the relation between cocaine abuse and violence: neurological facilitation mechanisms. Adicciones. 2015;27(1):64–74. Published 2015 Mar 1. [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  31. Sander AM, Fuchs KL, High WM, Jr, Hall KM, Kreutzer JS, Rosenthal M. The Community Integration Questionnaire revisited: an assessment of factor structure and validity. Arch Phys Med Rehabil. 1999;80:1303–1308. doi: 10.1016/s0003-9993(99)90034-5. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  32. Spitzer RL, Gibbon ME, Skodol AE, Williams JBW, First MB. DSM-IV-TR Casebook: a Learning Companion to the Diagnostic and Statistical Manual of Mental Disorders, Text Rev. American Psychiatric Publishing, Inc; 2002. [Google Scholar]
  33. Sprengelmeyer R, Rausch M, Eysel UT, Przuntek H. Neural structures associated with recognition of facial expressions of basic emotions. Proc R Soc Lond Ser B Biol Sci. 1998;265:1927–1931. doi: 10.1098/rspb.1998.0522. [DOI] [PMC free article] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  34. Thompson AE, Voyer D. Sex differences in the ability to recognise non-verbal displays of emotion: a meta-analysis. Cogn Emot. 2014;28:1164–1195. doi: 10.1080/02699931.2013.875889. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  35. Trubanova A, Kim I, Coffman MC, Bell MA, Richey JA, LaConte SM, Gracanin D, White SW. The role of perspective-taking on ability to recognize fear. Curr Res Psychol. 2016;6:22–30. doi: 10.3844/crpsp.2015.22.30. [DOI] [PMC free article] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  36. Turner TH, LaRowe S, Horner MD, Herron J, Malcolm R. Measures of cognitive functioning as predictors of treatment outcome for cocaine dependence. J Subst Abuse Treat. 2009;37:328–334. doi: 10.1016/j.jsat.2009.03.009. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  37. Verdejo-García A, Bechara A. A somatic marker theory of addiction. Neuropharmacology. 2009;56:48–62. doi: 10.1016/j.neuropharm.2008.07.035. [DOI] [PMC free article] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  38. Volkow ND, Ding Y-S, Fowler JS, Wang G-J, Logan J, Gatley JS, Dewey S, Ashby C, Liebermann J, Hitzemann R, Wolf AP. Is methylphenidate like cocaine?: studies on their pharmacokinetics and distribution in the human brain. Arch Gen Psychiatry. 1995;52:456–463. doi: 10.1001/archpsyc.1995.03950180042006. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  39. Wardle MC, Garner MJ, Munafò MR, de Wit H. Amphetamine as a social drug: effects of d-amphetamine on social processing and behavior. Psychopharmacology (Berl) 2012;223:199–210. doi: 10.1007/s00213-012-2708-y. [DOI] [PMC free article] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  40. Williams CT, Latkin CA. Neighborhood socioeconomic status, personal network attributes, and use of heroin and cocaine. Am J Prev Med. 2007;32:S203–S210. doi: 10.1016/j.amepre.2007.02.006. [DOI] [PMC free article] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  41. Wright DB, Sladden B. An own gender bias and the importance of hair in face recognition. Acta Psychol (Amst) 2003;114:101–114. doi: 10.1016/s0001-6918(03)00052-0. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  42. Zimet GD, Dahlem NW, Zimet SG, Farley GK. The multidimensional scale of perceived social support. J Pers Assess. 1988;52:30–41. doi: 10.1080/00223891.1990.9674095. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]

Associated Data

This section collects any data citations, data availability statements, or supplementary materials included in this article.

Supplementary Materials

Supplementary Materials

RESOURCES